Correlatos cognitivos y neuropsicológicos de los cinco grandes : una revisión en el área de la neurociencia de la personalidad
Universidad Cooperativa de Colombia
email: jorge.restrepoc@campusucc.edu.co
Introducción: la neurociencia de la personalidad es un área de estudio de origen relativamente reciente. Objetivo: estudiar los fundamentos neurobiológicos y neuropsicológicos de la personalidad.
Metodología: este es un artículo de revisión narrativa en el cual se presentan y discuten algunas, quizás la mayoría, de las investigaciones realizadas en esta área. La estructura del artículo está en función de los cinco grandes factores de la personalidad. Para cada factor (neuroticismo, extraversión, apertura, amabilidad y responsabilidad), se revisa y discute la literatura existente en relación con los correlatos neuropsicológicos en la función ejecutiva, la memoria y la inteligencia.
Resultados: por ser un área de estudio reciente y a razón del tipo de revisión llevado a cabo, no es posible realizar una conclusión precisa, es decir, no es posible establecer, en el estado del arte actual, cómo se correlacionan los factores y las facetas de la personalidad con la función ejecutiva, la inteligencia y la memoria.
Conclusión: en términos generales, puede afirmarse que existe un perfil neuropsicológico diferente para cada uno de los factores.
Ahadi, S. A. y Rothbart, M. K. (1994). En C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm y R. P. Martin (Eds.), The Developing Structure of Temperament and Personality from Infancy to Adulthood (pp. 189–207). Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Laurence Erlbaum.
Alperson, B. D. (1968). Color association values and response interference on variants of the Stroop test. Acta Psychologica, 26(3), 286-295.
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Vernon, P. A. y Jang, K. L. (2000). Fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and the openness/intellect factor. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(2), 198–207.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Bagby, R. M. y Parker, D. A. (2001). Relation of rumination and distraction with neuroticism and extraversion in a sample of patients with major depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25(1), 91–102.
Baker, J. y Bichsel, J. (2006). Personality predictors of intelligence: Differences between young and cognitively healthy older adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(5), 861–871.
Bartels, M., van Weegen, F. I., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Carlier, M., Tinca J.C. Polderman, T. J. C., Hoekstra, R. A. y Boomsma, D. I. (2012). The five factor model of personality and intelligence: A twin study on the relationship between the two constructs. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 368-373.
Belojevic, G., Jakovljevic, B. y Slepcevic, V. (2003). Noise and mental performance: Personality attributes and noise sensitivity. Noise and Health, 6(21), 77-89.
Berggren, N., Koster, E. W. H. y Derakshan, N. (2012). The effect of cognitive load in emotional attention and trait anxiety: An eye-movement study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 79 – 91.
Bermúdez, J., Pérez, A. M. y Padilla, M. (1988). Extraversion and task properties as determinants of incidental recall. European Journal of Personality, 2(1), 57–65.
Bettencourt, B. A., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J. y Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and Aggressive Behavior Under Provoking and Neutral Conditions: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 751–777.
Bishop, S. J. (2007) Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 307-16.
Bishop, S.J. (2009) Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience 12, 92-98.
Bono, J. E. y Vey , M. A. (2007). Personality and emotional performance: extraversion, neuroticism, and self-monitoring. Journal of Occupational and Health Psychology, 12(2), 177-192.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. y Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.
Boyle, L. L., Lyness, J. M., Duberstein, P. R., Karuza, J., King, D. A., Messing, S., y Tu, X. (2010). Trait Neuroticism, Depression, and Cognitive Function in Older Primary Care Patients. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(4), 305–312.
Brebner, J. (1985). Personality theory and movement. En B. D. Kirkcaldy (Ed.), Individual differences in movement (pp. 27-41). Lancaster, England: mtp Press Limited.
Brebner, J. M. (1980). Reaction time in personality theory. En A. T. Welford (Ed.), Reaction times (pp. 309-320). London, England: Academic Press.
Bredemeier, K., Berenbaum, H., Most, S. B. y Simons, D.J. (2011). Links between neuroticism, emotional distress, and disengaging attention: Evidence from a single-target rsvp task. Cognition and Emotion, 25(8),1510-1519.
Buela Casal, G., Caballo, V. E., García Cueto, E. y Flores Cubos, P. (1990). Attention and reaction time differences in introversion-extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 11(2), 195-197.
Campbell, A. M., Davalos, D. B., McCabe, D. P. y Troup, L. J. (2011). Executive functions and extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(6), 720–725.
Campbell, L., Simpson, J., Stewart, M. y Manning, J. (2003). Putting Personality in Social Context: Extraversion, Emergent Leadership, and the Availability of Rewards. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(12), 1547-1559.
Carver, C. S. y White, T. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The bis/bas scales. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67(2), 319-333.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T. y Furnham, A. (2003). Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples, Journal of Research in Personality, 37(4), 319-338.
Cherkes-Julkowski, M. (2005). The dysfunctionality of executive function. Kearney, NE, usa: Morris Publishing.
Colom, R. y Quiroga, M. A. (2009). Neuroticism, intelligence, and intra-individual variability in elementary cognitive tasks: Testing the mental noise hypothesis. Psicothema, 21(3), 403-408.
Costa, P. T. y McCrae, R. (1992). The neo pi-r professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assesment Resources.
Damasio, A. R. (1996). Descartes’ error. Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.
Davis, R. N. y Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive Inflexibility Among Ruminators and Nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(6), 699–711.
Denburg, N. L., Weller, J. A., Yamada, T. H., Shivapour, D. M., Kaup, A. R., LaLoggia, A., Cole, C. A., Tranel, D. y Bechara, A. (2009). Poor decision making among older adults is related to elevated levels of neuroticism. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 164-72.
Depue, R. A. y Iacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 457–492.
Derakshan, N. y Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance: new developments from attentional control theory. European Psychologist, 14(2), 168–176.
Derakshan, N., Smyth, S. y Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Effects of state anxiety on perfor-mance using a task-switching paradigm: an investigation of attentional control theory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(6), 1112–1117.
DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Impulsivity as a personality trait. En K. D. Vohs y R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2.ed., pp. 485-504). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., y Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: Cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73(4), 825–858.
Dreisbach, G. y Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30(2), 343–353.
Duncker, K. (1945). On Problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5). i-113.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, F. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Smith, M. y Maszk, P. (1996). The relations of children’s dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 195–209.
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological bases of personality. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Eysenck, M. W. y Derakshan, N., (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(7), 955–960.
Eysenck, M.W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R. y Calvo, M.G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353.
Eysenck, M.W., Payne, S. y Derakshan, N. (2005). Trait anxiety, visuospatial processing, and working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 19(8), 1214–1228.
Feldman Barrett, L. (1997). The relationship among momentary emotion experiences, personality descriptions, and retrospective ratings of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(10), 1100-1110.
Flehmig, H. C., Steinborn, M., Langner, R. y Westhoff, K. (2007). Neuroticism and the mental noise hypothesis: Relationships to lapses of attention and slips of action in everyday life. Psychology Science, 49(4), 343-360.
Flory, J. D., Harvey, P. D., Mitropoulo, V., New, A. S., Silverman, J. M., Siever, L. S. y Manuck, S. (2006). Dispositional impulsivity in normal and abnormal samples. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40(5), 438–447.
Frost, P., Sparrow, S. y Barry, J. (2006). Personality Characteristics Associated with Susceptibility to False Memories. The American Journal of Psychology, 119(2), 193-204.
Furnham, A., Forde, L., y Cotter, T. (1998). Personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 24(2), 187–192.
Gabrys, J. B., Schumph, D. y Utendale, K. A. (1987). Shortterm memory for two meaningful stories and self-report on the Adult Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Psychological Reports, 61(1), 51-59.
García, D. y Muñoz, P. (2000). Funciones ejecutivas y rendimiento escolar en educación primaria. Un estudio exploratorio. Revista Complutense de Educación, 11(1), 39–56.
Goel, V. y Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal lobes implicated in ‘planning’ functions? Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia, 33(5), 623–642.
Goldstein, D. G. y Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109(1), 75–90.
Gray, J. (2009). The association between empathy, the Big 5 Dimensions of personality and prosocial behaviour: What causes individuals to act prosocially? Word Journal Of The International Linguistic Associatio (Tesis de pregrado, Universidad de Edinburgo).
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septohippocampal system. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288.
Gray, J. R. y Braver, T. S. (2002). Personality predicts working-memory-related activation in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 64–75.
Gupta, B. S. y Kumar, J. (1990). Effects of extraversion and elaboration of encoding on immediate free recall and recognition performance. Psychological Studies, 35(1), 46–51.
Harley, T. A. y Matthews, G. (1992). Interactive effects of extraversion, arousal and time of day on semantic priming: Are they pre-lexical or post-lexical? Personality and Individual Differences, 13(9), 1021–1029.
Helode, R. D. (1982). Cognitive and non cognitive correlate of the Stroop interference effect. Journal of Psychological Researches, 26(3), 142-145.
Hervas, G. y Vazquez, C. (2011). What Else Do You Feel When You Feel Sad? Emotional Overproduction, Neuroticism and Rumination. Emotion, 11(4), 881-895.
Hilbig, B. E. (2008). Individual differences in fast-and-frugal decision making: Neuroticism and the recognition heuristic. Journal of Research in Personality, 42 (5), 1641-1645.
Holland, D. C., Dollinger, S. J., Holland, C. J. y MacDonald, D. A. (1995). The relationship between psychometric intelligence and the five-factor model of personality in a rehabilitation sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(1), 79–88.
Horn, J. L. (1985). Remodeling old models of intelligence. En B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp.267–300). New York: Wiley.
Howarth, E. y Eysenck, H. J. (1968). Extraversion, arousal and paired-associates recall. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 3(2), 114-116.
Huertas-Martínez, J. A., Ardura, A., Fernández-Alcaraz, C. y García-Andres, E. (2010). Emotional regulation and executive function profiles of functioning related to the social development of children. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 2077-2081.
Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders:Review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156−176.
Isen, A. M. (1984). The influence of positive affect on decision making and cognitive organization. Advances in Consumer Research, 11, 534-537.
Isen, A. M. y Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1206-1217.
Isen, A. M. y Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. Social Cognition, (2), 18-31.
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A. y Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122-1131.
Isen, A. M., Niedenthal, P. M. y Cantor, N. (1992). An influence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation and Emotion, 16(1), 65-78.
Jelicic, M., Bosma, H., Ponds, R. W. H. M., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Houx, P. J. y Jolles, J. (2003). Neuroticism does not affect cognitive functioning in later life. Experimental Aging Research, 29(1), 73–78.
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Rosseli, M., Workman, K. A., Santisi, M., Rios, J. D. y Bojan, D. (2002). Agreeableness, conscientiousness and effortful control processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(5), 476–489.
Jensen, A. R. (1992). The importance of intraindividual variability in reaction time. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(8), 869-882.
Jensen, A. R. (2007). Clocking the mind. The Netherlands: Elsevier.
Jorm, A. F., Mackinnon , A. J., Christensen, H., Henderson, S., Scott, R. y Korten, A. (1993). Cognitive functioning and neuroticism in an elderly community sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(6), 721–723.
Joseph, R. M. y Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopatholgy, 16(1), 137-55.
Koelega, H. S. (1992). Extraversion and vigilance performance: 30 years of inconsistencies. Psychological Bulletin, 112(2), 239-258.
Koster, E. H., Raedt, R. D., Goeleven, E., Franck, E. y Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: Maintained attention to and delayed engagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446-455.
Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M. y Benton, A. L. (1991). Frontal lobe function and dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Introversion and working memory: central executive differences. Personality and Individual Dfferences, 28(3), 479-486.
Lieberman, M. D. y Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why introverts can’t always tell who likes them: Multitasking and nonverbal decoding. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80(2), 294-310.
Lounsbury, J. W., Welsh, D. P., Gibson, L. W. y Sundstrom, E. (2005). Broad and narrow personality traits in relation to cognitive ability in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(5),1009–1019.
Luu, P., Collins, P. y Tucker, D. M. (2000). Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: Negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 43–60.
Lyubomirsky, S. y Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,69(1), 176–190.
Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D. y Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 166–177.
Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D. y Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 1041–1060.
Matthews, G., Davies, D. R. y Lees, J. L. (1990). Arousal, extraversion, and individual differences in resource availability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 150-168.
McCrae, R. R. (1993–1994). Openness to Experience as a basic dimension of personality. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 13(1), 39–55.
Merema, M. R., Speelman, C. P., Foster, J. K. y Kaczmarek, E. A. (2013). Neuroticism (Not Depressive Symptoms) Predicts Memory Complaints in Some Community-Dwelling Older Adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(8), 729-736.
Muris, P. E., Bos, A. E., Mayer, B. N., Verkade, R., Thewissen, V. y Dell’Avvento, V. (2009). Relations among behavioral inhibition, Big Five personality factors, and anxiety disorder symptoms in non-clinical children. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(4), 525-529.
Muris, P. E., de Jong, P. J. y Engelen, S. (2004). Relationships between neuroticism, attentional control, and anxiety disorders symptoms in non-clinical children. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(4), 789–797.
Muris, P., Roelofs, J., Rassin, E., Franken, I. y Mayer, B. (2005). Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(6), 1105–1111.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504–511.
O’Brien, T. B. y DeLongis, A. (1996). The interactional context of problem-, emotion-, and relationship- focused coping: The role of the Big Five personality factors. Journal of Personality, 64(4), 775−813.
Ode, S. y Robinson, M. D. (2007). Agreeableness and the self-regulation of negative affect: Findings involving the neuroticism/somatic distress relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(8), 2137-2148.
Pearson, P. R. (1993). Cognitive functioning and neuroticism in elderly psychiatric patients. Personality and Individual Differences, 14(1), 265–266.
Portella, M. J., Goodwin, G. M., Flint, J., Cowen, P. y Harmer, C. J. (2004). Neuroticism as a psychological vulnerability factor for mood disorders. Journal of Psychopharmacology 18(3), A12-A12.
Posner, P. I. y Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.
Puskar, A. (2011). Investigation the relationship between the cognitive and affective components of empathy and frontal lobe functioning in college students (Doctoral Dissertation, Indiana University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania).
Rabin, L. A., Fogel, J. y Nutter-Upham, K. E. (2011). Academic procrastination in college students: The role of self-reported executive function. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 344-357.
Ramchandran, K. (2011). The neuropsychological correlates of leadership effectiveness. Theses and Dissertations. Iowa: University of Iowa.
Rameson, L. T. y Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Empathy: A Social Cognitive Neuroscience Approach. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(1), 94–110.
Rasmussen, C., Wyper, K. y Talwar, V. (2009). The relation between theory of mind and executive functions in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Canadian Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 16(2), e370-80.
Reeve, W. V. y Schnadler, S. L. (2001). Frontal lobe functioning in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Adolescence, 36(144), 749–765.
Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E. y Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability to episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22(4), 401–423.
Roberts, M. J. (2002). The relationship between extraversion and ability. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 517–522.
Robinson, M. D. y Tamir, M. (2005). Neuroticism as mental noise: a relation between neuroticism and reaction time standard deviations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 107–114.
Rothbart, M. K. y Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. Handbook of child psychology. En N. Eisenberg & W.Damon (Eds.), Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 99-166). New York: Wiley.
Safer, M. A. y Keuler, D. J. (2002). Individual differences in misremembering pre-psychotherapy distress: Personality and memory distortion. Emotion, 2(2), 162-178.
Safer, M. A., Levine, L. J. y Drapalski, A. L. (2002). Distortion in Memory for Emotions: The Contributions of Personality and Post-Event Knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(11), 1495-1507.
Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M. y Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(4), 566–594.
Sanford, L. C. y Fisk, J. E. (2009). How does the extraversion personality trait influence false recall with the Deese–Roediger–McDermott (drm) paradigm? Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 972-977.
Schretlen, D. J., Van der Hulst, E. J., Pearlson, G. D. y Gordon, B. (2010). A neuropsychological study of personality: trait openness in relation to intelligence, fluency, and executive functioning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(10), 1068-1073.
Schwartz, S. (1979). Differential effects of personality on access to various longterm memory codes. Journal of Research in Personality, 13(4), 396–403.
Smits, D. J. y Boeck, P. D. (2006). From BIS/BAS to the Big Five. European Journal of Personality, 20(4), 255–270.
Stahl, J. y Gibbons, H. (2007). Dynamics of response-conflict monitoring and individual differences in response control and behavioral control: An electrophysiologicalinvestigation using a stop-signal paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(3), 581-596.
Stahl, J. y Rammsayer, T. (2007). The point of no return in motor response: Extraversion-related differences. Psychology Science, 49(1), 58-67.
Stankov, L. (2009). Conservatism and cognitive ability. Intelligence, 37, 294–304.
Stough, C., Brebner, J., Nettelbeck, T., Cooper, C. J., Bates, T. y Mangan, G. L. (1996). The relationship between intelligence, personality, and inspection time. British Journal of Psychology, 87(2), 255–268.
Studer-Luethi, B., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M. y Perrig, W. J. (2012). Influence of neuroticism and conscientiousness on working memory training outcome. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(1), 44–49.
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F. y Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–322.
Teasdale, J. D. y Green, H. A. (2004). Ruminative self-focus and autobiographical memory. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(8), 1933–1943.
Tobin, R., Graziano, W., Vanman, E. y Tassinary, L. (2000), Personality, emotional experience, and efforts to control emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 656-69.
Wainwright, M. A., Wright, M. J., Luciano, M., Geffen, G. M. y Martin, N. G. (2008). Genetic covariation among facets of openness to experience and general cognitive ability. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 11(3), 275-286. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/twin.11.3.275
Wallace, J. E. y Newman, J. P. (1998). Neuroticism and the facilitation of automatic orienting of attention. Personality and Individual Differences, 24(2), 253-266. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00153-0
Watkins, E., y Moulds, M. L. (2005). Distinct modes of ru-minative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion5(3), 319-328. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319
Atribución: Esta licencia permite a otros distribuir, mezclar, ajustar y construir a partir de su obra, incluso con fines comerciales, siempre que le sea reconocida la autoría de la creación original. Esta es la licencia más servicial de las ofrecidas. Recomendada para una máxima difusión y utilización de los materiales sujetos a la licencia.